The Social Brain: Critical Perspectives on Science, Society and Neurodiversity
Part 1
Part 2
Without a sufficiently well-developed theory, then it is hard to interpret results in a meaningful way.
Anything that turns up as statistically significant can be used to tell a story or sell newspapers.
At most, only 0.4% of adolescent wellbeing is related to screen use - which only slightly surpasses the negative effect of regularly eating potatoes.
Predictions are often impossible to falsify because of infinite wriggle room…
Inputs to informative hypothesis tests:
What do we mean by terms such as screen time, intrinsic motivation or depression?
Why is this important?
Measurement schmeasurement
We demonstrate that psychology is plagued by a measurement schmeasurement attitude: questionable measurement practices are common, hide a stunning source of researcher degrees of freedom, pose a serious threat to cumulative psychological science, but are largely ignored.
[[some stuff here describing the problems]]
Once concepts are defined, we need a causal model of how they relate to each other.
A good theory is clear about its boundary conditions – where does the theory apply and where is beyond its scope?
OK, these seem like sensible ideas. But they are not new ideas?
Paul Meehl was writing about this in (1967).
Question for the class:
I’ll provide two examples:
Psychological theory tends to be narrative
Predictions are ordinal
Formal theory is a mathematical description that can give rise to quantitative predictions
It forces researchers to be explicit about parts of the system under investigation and how they are linked together (e.g., \(E = mc^2\)).
The benefit of formal theory here is not that our model is “correct”, but that it is explicit and it can therefore be more easily be falsified.
Formal theory reduces verbal wriggle room
Part 1
Part 2
Unless otherwise specified, icons and images were used under license from The Noun Project
ETH Zürich | 376-1309-00 | The Social Brain